Apositive outlook–belief in your own future–is a trait associated with greater resilience. But who has got a firmer grip on reality: the glass-half-full optimist or the cold-eyed pessimist who focuses on obstacles ahead?
That’s what psychologists Lauren Alloy and Lyn Abramson set out to find in the late 1970s when they began a seminal series of experiments exploring how mood affects the way people think. Back then most therapists took the Freudian view that depressed people–and by extension, pessimists–were out of touch with reality. It made sense, since depression was considered an aberrant mental state. Their work and subsequent research show that the relationship between cognition and mood is far more complex.
In carefully designed experiments, Alloy, then at Northwestern University, and Abramson, then at the State University of New York at Stony Brook, sat students in front of a panel featuring a green light and a button that they were told would activate the light when pressed. In fact, the amount of control students had over the light varied from 0% to 100%, with many points in between. When they were asked how much control they thought they had over the light, the answers surprised the psychologists. Optimistic types (who scored low on tests for depressive symptoms) consistently overestimated their influence. By a lot. On average they believed they had 60% control even in sessions in which their button pressing had purely random effects. “The nondepressed had an illusion of control when in fact they had none,” says Alloy. By contrast, more pessimistic students (those who had more depressive symptoms) judged their performance more accurately.
The finding that depressive types were “sadder but wiser,” as the researchers put it, rocked conventional thinking in psychology. “Our demonstration was provocative because it was counter to theories of psychopathology,” Abramson says. Clearly, nondepressed individuals were just as capable of distorting reality as the mentally ill were supposed to be.
In a second series of experiments with the light panel, the researchers tried to prove that mood really was the deciding factor in how students performed. To do so, they induced a depressed mood in nondepressed students by asking them to read a series of increasingly negative statements, beginning with a neutral idea such as “Today is neither better nor worse than any other day” and ending up with “I’d like to go to sleep and never wake up again.” Similarly, they lifted the mood of depressed students by having them read positive statements. Sure enough, the students who were artificially “depressed” made more accurate judgments about their control over the light than students whose mood was happified. “Basically, we could flip the entire phenomenon around,” says Alloy.
Were happier folks deluded only about their power to control, or did they have similar illusions about other people? Alloy and Abramson tested that and found that happy people were much more accurate when observing others performing the light-panel task. The result was the opposite, however, with depressed people, who tended to overrate other people’s performance. “That fits the theory that depressed people have a poor view of themselves but a better view of others,” says Abramson.
In the years since Alloy and Abramson published their work, other researchers have built on their findings about how mood and outlook color all sorts of thinking tasks: making purchasing decisions, doing creative work, recognizing faces. In studies at the University of Utah, Lisa Aspinwall has found that when presented with an unsolvable problem, optimists spend less time on strategies that don’t produce results. Pessimists, on the other hand, tend to ensnare themselves in one approach, sticking with it despite its futility. “Optimists didn’t waste time and energy on something that couldn’t be done, and these coping skills may be what helps them to handle stressful events in life situations as well,” she says.
That doesn’t mean that pessimists are always at a disadvantage. Their negative filter can be advantageous in situations involving risk or critical judgment, such as buying a new home or filing taxes. So if you’re picking an architect, it might be a good idea to go with an optimist, but you may want a pessimist for an accountant.
More Must-Reads from TIME
- How Kamala Harris Knocked Donald Trump Off Course
- Introducing TIME's 2024 Latino Leaders
- George Lopez Is Transforming Narratives With Comedy
- How to Make an Argument That’s Actually Persuasive
- What Makes a Friendship Last Forever?
- 33 True Crime Documentaries That Shaped the Genre
- Why Gut Health Issues Are More Common in Women
- The 100 Most Influential People in AI 2024
Contact us at letters@time.com